Copyright © 2004 Society for Music Theory
3.1 Past Research
[1] Most claims about chord-tone doubling probably arise from inspection of musical scores. To our knowledge, however, only two theorists have supported their claims with quantitative evidence. McHose, in 1947, tallied the doubling of vocal parts and triad members in Bach’s chorale harmonizations. Huron, in 1993, modeled the doubling of scale degrees in the same repertoire.
McHose (1947)
[2] McHose studied doubling in the chorale harmonizations of Bach. His results, presented as statistical summaries, showed, for instance, that the root is most often doubled in root-position chords and that the soprano is most often doubled in first inversion.(70) Our analyses provide limited support for his findings.
Huron (1993)
[3] In contrast to McHose, the doubling rules Huron chose to study were of the scale-degree variety.(20) The repertoire under examination was similar, however: 50 chorale harmonizations by Bach—44 in major mode, and 6 in minor.
[4] To test the idea that Bach’s doublings favored strong or key-enhancing scale degrees, Huron quantified key enhancement using the Krumhansl-Kessler key profiles. (See §6.2.1.) Whereas traditional scale-degree rules are quite broad—e.g., don’t double the leading tone—the Krumhansl-Kessler profiles suggest finer distinctions; for example, the profiles suggest that the mediant will be doubled more often than the subdominant.
[5] Huron found that the Krumhansl-Kessler profiles provided a very close match to the distribution of doubled scale degrees. Although the non-doubled scale degrees also fit the profile, the fit of the doubled scale degrees was consistently better.
[6] Huron interpreted these results to mean that Bach intended to enhance key perception, and did so by doubling tones with a high score in the Krumhansl-Kessler profiles. Although Huron’s interpretation was reasonable, at least two alternatives can be proposed:
- Perhaps Bach sought to double certain chord members—which often happened to coincide with tones that had high Krumhansl-Kessler scores.
- On the other hand, perhaps Bach avoided doubling tendency tones, because doubling them would lead to clumsy voice-leading. Tendency tones—the leading tone and chromatics—invariably have low scores in the Krumhansl-Kessler profiles.
[7] In light of our own results, the last interpretation seems the most plausible account of Huron’s findings. (See §6.2.)
Copyright Statement
Copyright © 2004 by the Society for Music Theory. All rights reserved.
[1] Copyrights for individual items published in Music Theory Online (MTO) are held by their authors. Items appearing in MTO may be saved and stored in electronic or paper form, and may be shared among individuals for purposes of scholarly research or discussion, but may not be republished in any form, electronic or print, without prior, written permission from the author(s), and advance notification of the editors of MTO.
[2] Any redistributed form of items published in MTO must include the following information in a form appropriate to the medium in which the items are to appear:
This item appeared in Music Theory Online in [VOLUME #, ISSUE #] on [DAY/MONTH/YEAR]. It was authored by [FULL NAME, EMAIL ADDRESS], with whose written permission it is reprinted here.
[3] Libraries may archive issues of MTO in electronic or paper form for public access so long as each issue is stored in its entirety, and no access fee is charged. Exceptions to these requirements must be approved in writing by the editors of MTO, who will act in accordance with the decisions of the Society for Music Theory.
This document and all portions thereof are protected by U.S. and international copyright laws. Material contained herein may be copied and/or distributed for research purposes only.
Prepared by Brent Yorgason, Managing Editor and Tahirih Motazedian, Editorial Assistant