Rethinking Aural Skills Instruction through Cognitive Research: A Response
Elizabeth West Marvin
REFERENCE: https://mtosmt.org/issues/mto.21.27.2/mto.21.27.2.karpinski.html
REFERENCE: https://mtosmt.org/issues/mto.21.27.2/mto.21.27.2.chenette.html
REFERENCE: https://mtosmt.org/issues/mto.21.27.2/mto.21.27.2.gates.html
KEYWORDS: aural skills, pedagogy, curriculum, music cognition, solfège, key finding, auditory imagery
ABSTRACT: This essay responds to three papers appearing in this issue that relate music-cognitive research to aural skills pedagogy. Gary S. Karpinski focuses on tonic inference as support for do-based minor solfège pedagogy. My discussion supports this position, with evidence from key-profile experiments and corpus analyses. Timothy Chenette proposes a perceptually based learning sequence for aural skills instruction. He sketches a model curriculum, to which I propose a staffing solution and offer a research-based challenge: the high-voice superiority principle. Finally, Sarah Gates considers what the cognitive sciences can tell us about auditory imagery. I offer classroom strategies that take advantage of motor-area activation in the brain.
DOI: 10.30535/mto.27.2.4
Copyright © 2021 Society for Music Theory
[1.1] The three preceding essays address an important question often posed to music theorists whose research takes a cognitive bent: how can the explosive growth of research in music cognition inform aural skills pedagogy in a practical way? These authors attempt to answer that question. Gary S. Karpinski focuses on a well researched skill that is fundamental to musical transcription: tonic inference from an aural stimulus. Timothy Chenette invites us to consider a perceptually based learning hierarchy to shape aural skills curricula, rather than a topics-based sequence that mirrors the organization of harmony courses. Sarah Gates considers what the cognitive sciences can tell us about another skill fundamental to aural musicianship: auditory imagery, or the inward hearing of music.
[1.2] We begin with Karpinski’s paper, which revisits the well-trod question of do-based vs. la-based minor in movable-do solfège pedagogy as viewed through the lens of music-cognitive research. He argues first that tonic inference is a fundamental process of music cognition. Second, he demonstrates via musical examples that the full diatonic collection is not required for tonic to be perceived. Third, he shows how the pitch collection of a melody may unfold through time, gradually revealing its mode or scale, and he highlights the attendant problems this raises for solmization in the la-based system. As he points out, listeners don’t know whether “Norwegian Wood” is major or Mixolydian until nearly the end of its opening phrase; yet we can infer tonic earlier on, and therefore know that the tune begins on sol (2021, [17]–[22]). Once we know where do is, we can employ inflections within do-based minor to assign solmization to the tune, no matter the scale or mode. This is not so with la-based minor, he argues, which requires that the full collection be heard to assign the correct mode and solfège syllable for its final (and re for Dorian, mi for Phrygian, etc.). If instructors use solmization as a tool for melodic dictation—using, for example, the “proto-notation” method advocated by Karpinski 2000—then finding do and teaching students to assign solfège quickly and accurately is paramount.
[1.3] But what does music cognition tell us about extracting the tonic? How do we come to perceive this scale degree as most stable, as a perceptual anchor? Karpinski’s discussion begins with the cognitive principles proposed by Brown, Butler, and Jones 1994 and Butler 1989, in which the authors—building on Browne 1981—argue that the rare intervals of the diatonic collection (particularly the tritone and semitone) assist with aural position-finding within the scale. I have no doubt that this is one method by which tonic inference occurs. Others are at play as well, as Karpinski demonstrates in melodies without a complete diatonic collection and indeed without tritones and semitones, as in the opening to Wagner’s The Flying Dutchman (2021, Example 1). What he doesn’t mention is that Butler (1990) once engaged in a heated published exchange with music psychologist Carol Krumhansl over the question of how listeners determine the tonic when listening to music (Krumhansl 1990). Krumhansl’s critique relies on data from a series of experiments that use the probe-tone technique to measure listener perceptions of the relatedness of each pitch class to a given tonal context, by rating them from 1 to 7 (Krumhansl and Shepard 1979; Krumhansl and Kessler 1982). Results are shown as key profiles, which outline a tonal hierarchy—with the scale degrees of the tonic triad rated highest by listeners, the remaining diatonic tones next highest, and non-diatonic tones lowest.
[1.4] Importantly, Krumhansl (2001) demonstrates, through corpus analysis, a strong correlation between listener ratings of each scale degree and its frequency in Western tonal music repertoire. From this, we can extrapolate that listeners’ scale-degree perceptions are implicitly learned, from an unconscious tally of how often each scale degree occurs in the music to which we are exposed in daily life.(1) High listener ratings for and are arguably related to the higher frequency with which and are found in the corpus. Further, the profiles for parallel major and minor keys are strikingly similar, with and receiving the highest ratings, and small differences appearing only for modal scale degrees (for example, higher ratings for
[1.5] Implicit in this pedagogical discussion is the assumption that music principles studied here derive from Western art music. Even “Norwegian Wood” demonstrates structural principles not too far removed from the Western art-music canon that forms the basis of many music curricula. In an era when teachers are actively seeking to diversify the music considered in the theory classroom, we may need to consider how the principles we teach are dependent upon particular repertoires, and how pedagogical and cognitive strategies might differ if that repertoire were changed. For example, key-finding strategies might differ if popular music were included more systematically in our curricula. As corpus data from Temperley and de Clercq 2013 suggest, aural determination of major or minor mode might not be so clear in popular repertories, as Karpinski notes (2021, [6]). In recent popular music based on repeated harmonic loops, tonic inference can become quite challenging when loops suggest more than one possible tonic chord and thus an ambiguous key (see, for example, Richards 2017 and Clendinning 2018).(2) What other adjustments to our aural skills pedagogy might be necessitated by an infusion of popular and non-Western musics into the undergraduate curriculum?
[2.1] Timothy Chenette invites us to ponder which skills are “truly aural.” He suggests that truly aural skills are those that directly engage auditory working memory and attentional control.(3) Auditory working memory is the mental faculty that allows listeners to hold a musical idea in memory and to manipulate it—for example, by singing back with solfège syllables, altering a melody’s mode or rhythm in the mind, or remembering the beginning of a melody to transcribe it (while blocking out its continuation). Auditory attentional control comes into play when musicians direct their attention selectively to soprano or bass for harmonic dictation, or isolate the violin melody from an orchestral texture, or attend to cadences to identify phrase types. A number of researchers have compared musicians and nonmusicians in auditory tasks to assess their working memory and attentional control. As an example, Schulze, Mueller, and Koelsch (2011) asked participants to listen to five-note pitch patterns, half of which featured a prominent tonic triad and half of which were more ambiguous as to key. Listeners were asked to rehearse the melodies mentally for four to six seconds, and then were given a probe tone to identify as belonging to the sequence or not belonging. During the task, participants were scanned with fMRI neuroimaging. Musicians outperformed nonmusicians in both conditions; musicians (but not nonmusicians) performed significantly better on the tonal stimuli than the tonally ambiguous patterns. In addition—for musicians only—mental rehearsal of the patterns showed functional differences in brain areas known for strategy-based processing—again, in the tonal patterns, but not the ambiguous ones. In other words, during mental rehearsal, the musicians were actively seeking a strategy for encoding and remembering. Such working-memory strategies may well have been tonic-inference, scale-degree identification, and so on—strategies they were less able to apply to the tonally ambiguous stimuli. I find that this cognitive search for structure argues in favor of what Chenette (perhaps polemically) calls a “logical” ordering in aural skills curricula, at least during the early stages of training, when students are learning to hear and name fundamental patterns of tonal and rhythmic structure. But his point is well taken that we tend to ignore other auditory working-memory and attentional-control tasks that do not fit so neatly into a theory curriculum, such as listening for blend in an ensemble, memorizing canonical literature, identifying timbres and spatial characteristics of sound, and so on.
[2.2] Indeed, I am very much struck by the important musical skills Chenette has captured in his cross-disciplinary diagram of aural skills course topics—skills applicable to historical performance practice, jazz, ethnomusicology, music therapy, audio engineering, and more (2021, Example 8). I would suggest adding a popular-music area to the diagram, especially as curricula are diversifying repertoire and schools are creating commercial and popular-music majors. While some of the skills would be similar to those for jazz (memorization of canonical literature and style-specific improvisation) and for audio engineering (listening for sound quality, identifying filters, timbre analysis), the literature, progressions, and styles would differ. Everything on this chart is an aural skill truly relevant to the careers for which our students are preparing. Yet many of these skills are missing from our aural skills classes, and with musical relevance comes student buy-in and intrinsic motivation to learn (Ryan and Deci 2000; Marvin 2018). So, I am very sympathetic to the point he is making. I like the idea of incorporating logical curriculum units like “developing internal hearing” or “tuning and intonation.” But I would like to ask a polemical question myself: why is it that the music theory department owns aural skills training? Perhaps the final semester of an aural skills curriculum is the time to consider offering discipline-specific aural skills classes, designed and taught by faculty in those disciplines. Why not have the conducting faculty offer a skills class that includes multi-part error detection, clef transposition, polyphonic listening for blend and balance, and memorization and transcription of canonical literature? Why not have the ethnomusicology and jazz departments offer a skills class focused on the types of transcription and genre/style identification necessary for those disciplines? Why not have keyboard faculty teach sing-and-play sight reading, style-specific improvisation, error detection while accompanying a soloist, and instruction on hearing and conveying form? One can envision many more such discipline-specific aural skills courses, and by the fourth or fifth semester of study, students will be ready for them.
[2.3] Chenette’s Examples 2–5 accompany an extended discussion of harmonic dictation. He points out that this task “involves multiple simultaneous ‘voices,’ and it is well established that human attention can have a high degree of focus on only one object at a time” (2021, [2.4]). He goes on to focus on bass-line dictation as the preferred focus of attention. As widespread as this strategy is in aural skills classrooms, it flies in the face of music-cognitive research, which has reliably shown a “high-voice superiority” in homophonic textures (Marie et al. 2012; Marie and Trainor 2014; and Trainor et al. 2014). Researchers have shown through EEG studies that two-voice musical textures form two separate memory traces in the brain, and that the higher of the voices shows a larger response to changed pitches when compared with the lower voice—even in children as young as seven months old, suggesting that the high-voice superiority effect may be innate. Only years of practice on a bass-range musical instrument can mitigate this effect somewhat (Marie et al. 2012). Trainor et al. 2014 hypothesize that the high-voice superiority effect may be a by-product of the physiology of the auditory system. Huron 2016 suggests that this effect may be a feature of auditory masking. What does this mean for harmonic dictation? While dictation of the lowest voice does indeed provide necessary information for a Roman numeral analysis, students may struggle to focus auditory attention on the bass line, as this works against cognitive principles, acoustics, and possibly physiology. Further, upper voices tend to feature simpler stepwise motion while bass lines feature more skips and leaps. Together, these findings suggest that Chenette’s perceptually based learning hierarchy might ask students first to focus on the soprano voice in homophonic textures, before turning their attention to the bass and Roman numerals or functional symbols.
[3.1] We turn finally to auditory imagery, the ability to imagine sound that is not actually present. Sarah Gates asks whether aural skills classes—for all their talk of “inner hearing” and “audiation”—actually improve students’ ability to imagine sound.(4) To answer this question she introduces the Bucknell Auditory Imagery Scale (or BAIS) developed by psychologist Andrea Halpern (2015) and discusses expertise acquisition using a long-term working memory model. BAIS is a psychometric test that presents auditory scenes to be imagined and manipulated, with participants assessing their success at each task. Gates describes her ongoing research using BAIS as a pre- and post-semester measure for aural skills students and discusses ways teachers might focus explicitly on developing students’ auditory imagery. One way, which she calls “content acquisition,” is to teach students typical musical patterns and melodic-harmonic schemas to recognize and label—patterns that they will learn to hear inwardly. Another is semantic encoding, assignment of solfège or scale-degree numbers to what is heard, to improve the quality of the imagined sound. A third involves cuing strategies for auditory imagery, such as subvocalization (covert tension in the larynx that simulates singing, but in silence) and multimodal associations (such as imagined instrument playing, silent whistling, or imagined notation).
[3.2] These multimodal associations are neurologically based: neuroimaging shows a strong link between motor planning and auditory imagery. For example, Bangert et al. (2006) and Meister et al. (2004) demonstrate that a pianist imagining a piano melody will engage the motor areas of the brain necessary for its performance. It follows that moving the fingers may assist in creating a more robust auditory image, and Gates suggests that incorporating keyboard study into the aural skills classroom will strengthen students’ multimodal sound-to-action connections as well as their auditory imagery (2021, [2.6]). Similarly, Lévêque and Schön (2015) showed greater activations in motor areas responsible for singing when listeners heard sung melodies versus melodies in other timbres. This finding resonates well with the classroom challenge of students who have difficulty singing back pitch patterns or melodies played on the piano. For some students, performance improves when those melodic patterns are sung to them (rather than played). It may be simpler for them to sing back patterns from a sung model because the brain’s motor areas for singing have been activated while listening.
[3.3] Pfordresher, Halpern, and Greenspon (2015) have developed a multi-modal model that represents automatic associations between imagery and action. For singing, this mapping is between pitch height and regulation of vocal-fold tension. They posit that poor singers have deficits in the mapping between imagery and motor action; in particular, these singers tend to drift toward a preferred vocal range and a “comfort pitch.” As a practical application for the classroom, aural skills teachers should consider spending class time improving vocal technique: vocalizing our classes as one might warm up a choir, encouraging good posture and breath control, and working to expand non-singers’ vocal ranges. These authors also note the “somewhat puzzling” finding that poor singers have more difficulty singing back a single repeated pitch than they do a short pitch pattern (2015, 247). This should come as no surprise to an aural skills teacher, however. Isolated pitches have no musical context and thus cannot be assigned labels (like numbers or solfège) to assist with students’ auditory imagery. Three- and four-note patterns that arpeggiate tonic, dominant, and subdominant functions are the contextual building blocks of Edwin Gordon’s music learning sequence (2012). Importantly, each pattern is learned with the associated solfège syllables, in what Gates calls “semantic encoding of musical content” to improve auditory imagery and to free up memory to assist with multitasking, as would be necessary in improvisation (2021, [2.4] and [2.8]). In the classroom, students who are non-singers may have difficulty mapping musical patterns from non-vocal timbres (e.g., piano or experimental sine tones) to their voices, as suggested above by Lévêque and Schön 2015. Sung short pitch patterns, on the other hand, give students a tonal context that facilitates the transfer from imagery to motor action. Taken together, these studies suggest that motor activity—from subvocalization to instrumental fingering—should not be discouraged in the classroom as a “cheat” that substitutes for true auditory imagery; rather, this strategy should be embraced as a mimetic way of knowing, to enhance auditory imagery.
[3.4] A quarter century has passed since Butler and Lochstampfor considered the “bridges unbuilt” between music cognition and aural skills pedagogy (1993, 1). As the essays published here show, much progress has been made on both sides of the fence—both by music-cognitive researchers designing experiments with ecological validity and relevance to real music making, and by music theory pedagogues creating music curricula that respond to findings in the cognitive sciences. Looking back at the 1993 essay, I see how prescient these authors were as to the questions we are addressing today. They begin with two pedagogical vignettes that touch upon solfège methodologies, including la-based minor, as does Karpinski in the present issue. They also address auditory imagery—as Gates does here—asking “is there a motor-learning link, as well as a memory link at the cognitive level between the pedagogical devices of numbers or syllables and the clarity and strength of our mental images of tonal pitch relationships?” (Butler and Lochstampfor 1993, 8). Further, as the basis for planning aural skills curricula from the ground up, they pose questions strikingly similar to Chenette, such as: “Are solfege and dictation going to continue to be the staples in our students’ aural training regimen?
Elizabeth West Marvin
Eastman School of Music of the University of Rochester
26 Gibbs Street
Rochester, NY 14604
bmarvin@esm.rochester.edu
Works Cited
Bangert, Marc, Thomas Peschel, Gottfried Schlaug, Michael Rotte, Dieter Drescher, Hermann Hinrichs, Hans-Jochen Heinze, and Eckart Altenmüller. 2006. “Shared Networks for Auditory and Motor Processing in Professional Pianists: Evidence from fMRI Conjunction.” Neuroimage 30 (3): 917–26. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.10.044.
Brown, Helen, David Butler, and Mari Riess Jones. 1994. “Musical and Temporal Influences on Key Discovery.” Music Perception 11 (4): 371–407. https://doi.org/10.2307/40285632.
Browne, Richmond. 1981. “Tonal Implications of the Diatonic Set.” In Theory Only 5: 3–21.
Butler, David. 1989. “Describing the Perception of Tonality in Music: A Critique of the Tonal Hierarchy Theory and a Proposal for a Theory of Intervallic Rivalry.” Music Perception 6 (3): 219–42. https://doi.org/10.2307/40285588.
—————. 1990. “Response to Carol Krumhansl.” Music Perception 7 (3): 325–38. https://doi.org/10.2307/40285468.
Butler, David, and Mark Lochstampfor. 1993. “Bridges Unbuilt: Comparing the Literatures of Music Cognition and Aural Training.” Indiana Theory Review 14 (2): 1–17. https://www.jstor.org/stable/24045326.
Castellano, Mary A., Jamshed J. Bharucha, and Carol L. Krumhansl. 1984. “Tonal Hierarchies in the Music of North India.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 113 (3): 394–412. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.113.3.394.
Chenette, Timothy. 2018. “Reframing Aural Skills Instruction based on Research in Working Memory.” Journal of Music Theory Pedagogy 32: 3–20.
—————. 2021. “What Are the Truly Aural Skills?” Music Theory Online 27 (2). https://mtosmt.org/issues/mto.21.27.2/mto.21.27.2.chenette.html.
Clendinning, Jane Piper. 2018. “Despacito (2017) through a Music Analyst’s Lens: Close Analysis of a Global Hit.” Paper presented at CITYMAC (Society for Music Analysis Conference), London, UK, July 6, 2018.
Gates, Sarah. 2021. “Developing Musical Imagery: Contributions from Pedagogy and Cognitive Science.” Music Theory Online 27 (2). https://mtosmt.org/issues/mto.21.27.2/mto.21.27.2.gates.html.
George, Elyse M., and Donna Coch. 2011. “Music Training and Working Memory: An ERP Study.” Neuropsychologia 49 (5): 1083–94. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.02.001.
Gordon, Edwin E. 2012. Learning Sequences in Music: Skill, Content, and Patterns. GIA Publications.
Halpern, Andrea R. 2015. “Differences in Auditory Imagery Self-Report Predict Neural and Behavioral Outcomes.” Psychomusicology: Music, Mind, and Brain 25 (1): 37–47. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pmu0000081.
Hansen, Mads, Mikkel Wallentin, and Peter Vuust. 2013. “Working Memory and Musical Competence of Musicians and Non-Musicians.” Psychology of Music 41 (6): 779–93. https://doi.org/10.1177/0305735612452186.
Hubbard, Timothy L. 2010. “Auditory Imagery: Empirical Findings.” Psychological Bulletin 136 (2): 302–29. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018436.
Huron, David. 2006. Sweet Anticipation: Music and the Psychology of Expectation. MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/6575.001.0001.
—————. 2016. Voice Leading: The Science Behind a Musical Art. MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/9780262034852.001.0001.
Karpinski, Gary S. 2000. Aural Skills Acquisition: The Development of Music Listening, Reading, and Performing Skills in College-Level Musicians. Oxford University Press.
—————. 2021. “A Cognitive Basis for Choosing a Solmization System.” Music Theory Online 27 (2). https://mtosmt.org/issues/mto.21.27.2/mto.21.27.2.karpinski.html.
Krumhansl, Carol L. 1990. “Tonal Hierarchies and Rare Intervals in Music Cognition.” Music Perception 7 (3): 309–24. https://doi.org/10.2307/40285467.
—————. 2001. Cognitive Foundations of Musical Pitch Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195148367.001.0001.
Krumhansl, Carol L., and Edward J. Kessler. 1982. “Tracing the Dynamic Changes in Perceived Tonal Organization in a Spatial Representation of Musical Keys.” Psychological Review 89 (4): 334–68. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.89.4.334.
Krumhansl, Carol L., and Roger N. Shepard. 1979. “Quantification of the Hierarchy of Tonal Functions within a Diatonic Context.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 5 (4): 579–94. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.5.4.579.
Lévêque, Yohana, and Daniele Schön. 2015. “Modulation of the Motor Cortex during Singing-Voice Perception.” Neuropsychologia 70: 58–63. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.02.012.
Marie, Céline, Takako Fujioka, Leland Herrington, and Laurel J. Trainor. 2012. “The High-Voice Superiority Effect in Polyphonic Music is Influenced by Experience: A Comparison of Musicians Who Play Soprano-Range Compared with Bass-Range Instruments.” Psychomusicology: Music, Mind, and Brain 22 (2): 97–104. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030858.
Marie, Céline, and Laurel J. Trainor. 2014. “Early Development of Polyphonic Sound Encoding and the High Voice Superiority Effect.” Neuropsychologia 57: 50–58. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.02.023.
Marvin, Elizabeth West. 1995. “Research on Tonal Perceptions and Memory: What Implications for Music Theory Pedagogy.” Journal of Music Theory Pedagogy 9: 31&nash;70.
—————. 2007. “Absolute Pitch and the Pedagogy of Relative Pitch,” Journal of Music Theory Pedagogy 21: 1–34.
—————. 2018. “What I Know Now: Reflections on Music Theory Pedagogy.” In The Norton Guide to Teaching Music Theory, ed. Rachel Lumsden and Jeffrey Swinkin, 364–77. W.W. Norton.
Matsunaga, Rie, and Jun-ichi Abe. 2012. “Dynamic Cues in Key Perception.” International Journal of Psychological Studies 4 (1): 3–21. https://doi.org/10.5539/ijps.v4n1p3.
Meister, Ingo G., Timo Krings, Henrik Foltys, Babak Boroojerdi, Mareike Müller, Rudolf Töpper, and Armin Thron. 2004. “Playing Piano in the Mind—An fMRI Study on Music Imagery and Performance in Pianists.” Cognitive Brain Research 19 (3): 219–28. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2003.12.005.
Moreno, Jairo. 2003. “Subjectivity, Interpretation, and Irony in Gottfried Weber's Analysis of Mozart's ‘Dissonance’ Quartet.” Music Theory Spectrum 25 (1): 99–120. https://doi.org/10.1525/mts.2003.25.1.99.
Pallesen, Karen Johanne, Elvira Brattico, Christopher J. Bailey, Antti Korvenoja, Juha Koivisto, Albert Gjedde, and Synnöve Carlson. 2010. “Cognitive Control in Auditory Working Memory is Enhanced in Musicians.” PLOS ONE 5 (6): e11120, 1–12. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0011120.
Pfordresher, Peter Q., Andrea R. Halpern, and Emma B. Greenspon. 2015. “A Mechanism for Sensorimotor Translation in Singing: The Multi-Modal Imagery Association (MMIA) Model.” Music Perception 32 (3): 242–53. https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2015.32.3.242.
Richards, Mark. 2017. “Tonal Ambiguity in Popular Music’s Axis Progressions.” Music Theory Online 23 (3). https://doi.org/10.30535/mto.23.3.6.
Ryan, Richard M., and Edward L. Deci. 2000. “Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivations: Classic Definitions and New Directions.” Contemporary Educational Psychology 25 (1): 54–67. https://doi.org/10.1006/ceps.1999.1020.
Saslaw, Janna. 1990. “Gottfried Weber and Multiple Meaning.” Theoria 5: 74–103.
Schulze, Katrin, Karsten Müller, and Stefan Koelsch. 2011. “Neural Correlates of Strategy Use During Auditory Working Memory in Musicians and Non-Musicians.” European Journal of Neuroscience 33: 189-196. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07470.x.
Telesco, Paula J. 2013. “Teaching Elementary Aural Skills—How Current Brain Research May Help.” Journal of Music Theory Pedagogy 27: 211–46.
Temperley, David, and Trevor de Clercq. 2013. “Statistical Analysis of Harmony and Melody in Rock Music.” Journal of New Music Research 42 (3): 187–204. https://doi.org/10.1080/09298215.2013.788039.
Temperley, David, and Elizabeth West Marvin. 2008. “Pitch-Class Distribution and the Identification of Key.” Music Perception 25 (3): 193–212. https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2008.25.3.193.
Trainor, Laurel J., Céline Marie, Ian C. Bruce, and Gavin M. Bidelman. 2014. “Explaining the High Voice Superiority Effect in Polyphonic Music: Evidence from Cortical Evoked Potentials and Peripheral Auditory Models.” Hearing Research 308: 60–70. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.heares.2013.07.014.
Footnotes
1. In fact, listeners can also pick up scale-degree frequency distributions in unfamiliar repertoires fairly rapidly and without conscious effort, as Castellano, Bharucha, and Krumhansl (1984) showed with Western listeners exposed to music of North India. Temperley and Marvin (2008) demonstrated that listeners can infer the correct tonic and mode (major or minor) from constructed melodies that maintain the distributions of major- or minor-key profiles, but which present those tones in quasi-random order without schematic patterns of tonality (like ––).
Return to text
2. Karpinski quotes several authors whose discussions touch on key ambiguity: Butler 1989 in paragraph [5] and Matsunaga and Abe 2012 in [13]. In both cases, the described perceptual strategy is for the listener to determine a tonal center quite early in the piece and to keep it in place as a perceptual anchor to which all other tones relate, until another center “defeats it,” to use Butler’s words. I am struck by how similar this idea is to Gottfried Weber’s 1832 “Principle of Inertia” (see Saslaw 1990 and Moreno 2003 for further discussion). Matsunaga and Abe (2012) converge on language similar to Weber’s with their idea of “perceptual inertia.”
Return to text
3. A number of studies have found that musicians show superior auditory working memory and attentional control in comparison with nonmusicians in these areas (e.g., Pallesen et al. 2010; George and Coch 2011; Hansen, Wallentin, and Vuust 2013). We cannot infer a causal effect of music training on these abilities, however, since it may instead be that individuals with high abilities in these areas choose to pursue music training.
Return to text
4. For a comprehensive literature review of auditory imagery (published up to 2010) that includes not only musical imagery, but also imagery for speech and environmental sounds, see Hubbard 2010. Hubbard’s overview goes beyond the scope of our musical topic, but it provides helpful context for the study of auditory imagery in relation to perception and memory and also individual differences between people in their ability to imagine sound.
Return to text
5. The Journal of Music Theory Pedagogy has published a number of articles over the years that have brought together music cognition and music theory pedagogy. Among these are Chenette 2018; Marvin 2007, 1995; and Telseco 2013.
Return to text
Copyright Statement
Copyright © 2021 by the Society for Music Theory. All rights reserved.
[1] Copyrights for individual items published in Music Theory Online (MTO) are held by their authors. Items appearing in MTO may be saved and stored in electronic or paper form, and may be shared among individuals for purposes of scholarly research or discussion, but may not be republished in any form, electronic or print, without prior, written permission from the author(s), and advance notification of the editors of MTO.
[2] Any redistributed form of items published in MTO must include the following information in a form appropriate to the medium in which the items are to appear:
This item appeared in Music Theory Online in [VOLUME #, ISSUE #] on [DAY/MONTH/YEAR]. It was authored by [FULL NAME, EMAIL ADDRESS], with whose written permission it is reprinted here.
[3] Libraries may archive issues of MTO in electronic or paper form for public access so long as each issue is stored in its entirety, and no access fee is charged. Exceptions to these requirements must be approved in writing by the editors of MTO, who will act in accordance with the decisions of the Society for Music Theory.
This document and all portions thereof are protected by U.S. and international copyright laws. Material contained herein may be copied and/or distributed for research purposes only.
Prepared by Sam Reenan, Editorial Assistant
Number of visits:
4035